Reading: A Neglected Powerhouse
Spoken language is the mainstay of intervention for children on the spectrum. Even when they cannot speak,
the children receive hundreds of hours in speech/language therapy. Spoken language is deemed to be so critical
that any sounds—even those that just approximate words—are welcomed. The effort can go on for years—even when
the children are showing minimal, if any, progress.
By contrast, remarkably little work is put into the "other" form of language—that is, literacy (reading and
writing). Recently, when asked about what he is offering in literacy, the director of a school for children
4-10 years replied: "We don't have programs in that area. We have so many more important things to
teach." He is not alone. For many, literacy is not viewed as a critical skill.
Imagine what parents of neurotypical (NT) children would do if told, "Reading and writing are not in our
curriculum. There are just too many more important things your child has to learn." They know that literacy is
a, if not THE, single most valuable skill a child can master. It is the path to school success, jobs, and
intellectual growth. The consequences for failing to teach it to children on the spectrum are
incalculable.
A key factor responsible for this situation is the high percentage of children who are not
speaking or minimally verbal. Based on the course of development with NT children, it is assumed that spoken
language must precede written language. From this perspective, the children's limitations in spoken language
automatically place written language out of reach. "Why try?" since it is clearly "unattainable." But …
Reading Does Not Require Spoken Language
Moreover, literacy represents a powerful tool for enhancing the language abilities for all children on the
spectrum.
- Almost everything and anything that can be communicated via spoken language can be communicated via
written language. In other words, the benefits that flow from language can come from either modality—spoken
or written.
- Written language offers major advantages over spoken language. Children on the spectrum are highly visual
so that the visual print of the written page is more "user friendly." Unlike speech, it is set, stable and
unmoving – giving the child control over the pace at which the incoming information is processed (in
contrast to the rapid, relentless flow of auditory language).
- Unlike spoken language, children find written language to be appealing. There is a reason why so many
children—even at two and three years of age—love the little plastic letters that hang on refrigerators and
magnet boards. Our failure to take advantage of this motivation has had tremendous and unnecessary costs for
the children's development. We have essentially ignored another key path to language.
If you have doubts that non-speaking children can master written language, take a look at some of the books written by non-speaking individuals with autism such as "Ido in Autismland" and "Carly's Voice: Breaking through Autism." They show the amazing language skills that non-speaking children can attain.
New Ways of Instruction
To understand why ASD Reading works, it's important to understand why current methods are not working.
Traditional teaching methods rely on phonics which prevents the children from reaching any significant level
of skill. Phonics is grounded in spoken language via demands to produce sounds, to apply sophisticated
articulation, to segment and combine the sounds of spoken words and so on. The relentless push for sound
production makes reading unattainable.
By contrast, ASD Reading relies not on sounds but on vision—and it does so without using whole word or whole
language teaching. This method
uses "intensive word teaching" system where every component of a word is taught in a comprehensive and
integrated manner—its spelling, its meaning, its relationship to other words, its placement in sentences, etc.
These processes are applied even to the small, seemingly meaningless words like "some, but, and,
that," etc. Those words, which are often so weak in children with ASD, are critical to effective
language use and their mastery transforms the children's language skills.
Reading is commonly dissected into two major areas.
- decoding, or figuring out the words what the letters "say" and
- comprehension, or understanding the message that the words are saying.
The techniques and content in both areas are inappropriate for children on the spectrum. With their use, the
only outcome is the failure that we see pervading the lives of most of the children
Decoding: What Has Been Done and What Can Be Done
To understand decoding, it's easiest to start by focusing on a single, familiar phrase—"just sound it
out." This mantra of phonics instruction is pervasive—despite the fact that fewer than 1 in 5 words in
English can, in fact, be sounded out. The following sentence from a book designed for young readers
illustrates the problems:
Long, long ago when people lived in caves, some wolves began to wait nearby for the scraps of meat that people threw away.
(Wild, Wild Wolves by J. Milton, NY: Random House, 1992)
Try to put a single sound on each single letter. What you'll find is that you will arrive at the correct word
for only ONE of the 23 words. This situation leads to the second major component in phonics: namely, RULES.
For example, a common rule is that of the "double vowel" which states that when "two vowels are combined
(e.g., the ea in meat) they produce a single long vowel sound of the first vowel (e.g., eee). Given the
complexities of English, almost 600 rules are required to enable children to successfully decode the words
they have to grapple with from first to third grade alone.
The system poses problems for all children. Government figures over the past several decades consistently
show that difficulties with reading are found in approximately 2 out of 3 children.
While phonics causes neurotypical (NT) children to flounder, it usually places reading totally out of reach
for most children on the spectrum. With their limited spoken language, the instruction to "sound it out" is
not possible. Even if they can produce the individual sounds, they are not able to blend them together to form
words. And the application of long, complicated verbal rules to practically every word is not going to happen.
That's why so many schools, tied to phonics instruction, do not even try to teach reading to the children.
Instead they offer a curriculum confined to a few reading-related skills such as teaching children to write
their names and to memorize a few "sight words" linked to daily life functions.
None of this has to be! One of the clearest signs that alternatives are possible comes from the fascinating
phenomenon of "hyperlexia." This refers to the ability some children with ASD have to be amazingly successful
at decoding words—even when they do not understand the words that they are decoding and they have never seen
them before. Some four year old hyperlexic children can comfortably read the New York Times without skipping a
beat. In reading parlance, they have "broken" the code—without a moment's instruction in phonics and with no
training in the "rules" of phonics.
Although this remarkable phenomenon calls for far more analysis, it is clear that its attainment rests with
the strong visual processing abilities long noted in many children on the spectrum. These skills do not extend
to all visual information. For example, the children have difficulties processing social information such as
facial expressions. But, with non-social, unmoving inputs—including those containing complicated, detailed
networks, they can be comfortable, adept and motivated.
Hyperlexia was identified because the children spontaneously read aloud. Since those children were quite
verbal, observers concluded that it exists only in children who are "higher functioning." But it is not
limited to that group. Hyperlexia has not been recognized in children who are "lower functioning" because
their spoken language is limited or non-existent. Via specialized techniques (such as those you will find in
ASD Reading), we have found that many "non-speaking" children, prior to any instruction, can already read and
write a large number of words. It is the educational establishment's reliance on spoken language that has
prevented us from seeing their amazing set of skills.
The system's limitations should no longer hold us—or the children-captive. By altering the teaching techniques
to (a) capitalize on the children's strengths and (b) avoid those demanding complex, spoken language output,
it is possible to teach all children to decode effectively. That is what ASD Reading does.
Comprehension: What Has Been Done and What Can Be Done
The second major area of instruction—namely, comprehension—is marked by problems equal to those found in
teaching decoding. Despite its importance, remarkably little effort goes into helping children understand the
messages in the words they have decoded.
The basics of what happens are simple. After reading a segment of text, children are faced with questions
(spoken or written) that they have to answer. Questions such as "Who was the main character"? "What was he
looking for?" "Who helped him?" "What was his dog's name?" and so on. Often the final question is the
overarching demand for a summary: "Tell me what the story was about."
For children at the lower end of the spectrum, these sorts of questions are out of reach. While a few answers
may be correct, consistently effective responding is rarely seen. The problems are not limited to this group
since those at the higher end of the continuum also have difficulties. Even when they can answer the questions
about specifics, they cannot come up with the summary. That's why so many parents face the frustrating
situation of finding themselves saying, "His reading is fine—but he doesn't comprehend."
To see why this is happening, we need to consider a rarely discussed but issue that permeates the teaching of
reading. Because reading is language in written form, it is assumed that a child who has "good spoken
language" will simply—without instruction—transfer those skills to the written page and easily grasp what is
being "said." Unfortunately, that assumption is incorrect.
Consider, for example, a section of text for young readers. Like many of its genre, it is-- in an effort to
be simple-- composed of short words--many with only one syllable.
The tiny pig waddled across the pig pen. She squealed for attention. "Oi, oi." Her tiny tail curved over her body as she squeezed next to her mother on the trough….
No child, not even the most articulate one, speaks in this manner. This is not the language of everyday talk.
This comment applies to almost every book a child reads. Books for children present language that is vastly
different from the spoken language they hear and use. Nevertheless, it is assumed that because the children
use language effectively in speaking, they will do so in reading.
Some in the general (NT) population can deal with the chasm that exists between spoken and written language.
But a vast number cannot. That's why statistics on reading comprehension performance are so poor. They are
worse even than the figures on decoding.
For children on the spectrum, the situation is far more precarious. Their difficulties in producing and
comprehending spoken language have been extensively documented. Then without any solid preparation, they are
presented with the completely strange language used in writing. The failure that is steadily reported is only
to be expected.
In this process, many factors that have not even been on the radar screen become central. One, for example,
is the issue of sentence length. As is well known, the speech of many of the children is limited to sentences
that do not exceed three to four words.
What is less well known is that this length of production often reflects the length of language the children
can hold in mind. This means that they are unable to "take in" and comprehend any sentences that exceed that
number of words. Even a "simple" sentence such as "It is too warm today for a jacket." cannot be
successfully processed. For comprehension to occur, the children must be able to take in and retain long
stretches of language. This holds in both spoken and written language.
If the goal is competent language, it is vital to build up the processing of longer and longer sentences.
Fortunately, reading is actually a better pathway to this goal than speaking. Children who have speech
production problems cannot use expressive language to extend the length of their processing. Those limitations
do not apply to written language. Unlike speech where the auditory input rapidly disappears, in writing, the
visual input stays for as long as is needed. Through writing—a skill that receives major attention in ASD
Reading—the children are steadily empowered to advance into more complex language production and
processing.
Sentence length is but one of the many language components that play a key role in the content that ASD
Reading offers to the children. Many other aspects of language are controlled as well, including vocabulary
(understanding each word), verb tense (understanding the differences between present, past and future),
sentence forms (understanding the difference between statements and questions), and summarization (extracting
the core meaning of a set of sentences that convey a story).
ASD Reading provides unique content and techniques that lay the foundation for solid comprehension.